The Effectiveness of Mood Induction on Executive Functions and Theory of Mind in Female University Students

Document Type : پژوهشی

Authors

1 Master of Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Education and Psychology, Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Mashhad, Iran.

2 Professor, Department of Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Education and Psychology, Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Mashhad, Iran.

10.22067/tpccp.2025.76828.1292

Abstract

Introduction
Executive functions are a set of high-level cognitive functions, including planning, problem-solving, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and working memory. These functions enable individuals to perform goal-directed behaviors (Diamond, 2013; Bull & Lee, 2014). These functions are divided into two categories: cold and warm executive functions. Cold functions include cognitive processes such as working memory and inhibition that operate independently of emotions, while warm functions, such as theory of mind, are strongly linked to emotions and feelings (Peterson & Welsh, 2014; Anderson et al., 2008). Theory of mind, a constituent of warm executive function, pertains to an individual's capacity to ascribe mental states such as beliefs, desires, and intentions to oneself and others, leveraging this information to predict social behaviors (Ferguson & Austin, 2010). Conversely, mood, defined as a stable emotional state, has the capacity to influence cognitive functions, either positively or negatively. Positive mood has been linked to enhanced attention, memory, decision-making, and cognitive processing, leading to improved executive functions (Ashby et al., 1999; Davidson, 2003). Conversely, negative mood has been associated with diminished cognitive processing, attention, and mental flexibility (Clark & Purdon, 2004). Despite the large number of studies on the effectiveness of mood on cognition, no comprehensive study has examined the simultaneous effect of positive affective states (e.g., happiness and calm) and negative affective states (e.g., fear and anger) on cold and warm executive functions in an experimental framework. This theoretical and applied gap underscores the necessity for a study to elucidate the causal relationships between these variables. In addition, given the extant research evidence on gender differences in executive functions (Baron-Cohen et al., 2005; Herlitz & Rehnman, 2008; Miller & Halpern, 2014), the statistical population was selected from female students to increase homogeneity and strengthen the accuracy of the results.
Method
The present study was of a quasi-experimental nature, employing a pretest-posttest design with a control group. The statistical population comprised female students enrolled at Ferdowsi University of Mashhad in the second semester of the academic year 2020-2021. A convenience sampling method was employed to select 75 eligible individuals from among the volunteers, who were then randomly assigned to five groups of 15 (happiness, calmness, fear, anger, and a control group with neutral mood). The inclusion criteria encompassed individuals between the ages of 18 and 30 years, with the additional stipulation that they be in their final year of study. Furthermore, to ensure the integrity of the study's sample, participants were required to undergo a semi-structured interview to assess their mental and physical health. The instruments employed in the study included the Positive Affect and Negative Affect Scales (Watson et al., 1988), the N-Back test for measuring working memory (Kerchner, 1958), the Go/No-Go test for assessing cognitive inhibition (Hoffman, 1984), and Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test for measuring theory of mind (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). Mood induction was performed by showing targeted emotional films. Subsequent to the data collection, an analysis was conducted using multivariate analysis of covariance and Bonferroni post hoc test.
Findings
The results of a multivariate analysis of covariance demonstrated that positive mood induction significantly increased positive emotions (F=38.84, p<0.001, η²=0.696) and decreased negative emotions (F=71.19, p<0.001, η²=0.807). In the N-Back test, the happiness and calmness groups exhibited a substantial increase in the number of correct responses following the intervention (M=100.67 and M=114.36, respectively, F=16.98, p<0.001, η²=0.500), suggesting an enhancement in working memory. However, the reduction in reaction time in this test (F=0.91, p=0.343) was not significant. In the Go/No-Go test, cognitive inhibition performance in the happiness and relaxation groups exhibited a significant enhancement in correct responses (F=13.34, p<0.001, η²=0.443) and a reduction in incorrect responses (F=25.67, p<0.001, η²=0.605). Furthermore, a decline in reaction time was observed in the happiness and relaxation groups (F=4.11, p=0.047, η²=0.058). A significant discrepancy was identified between the groups in the theory of mind variable (F=4.32, p=0.004, η²=0.149), with the happiness group demonstrating the most substantial improvement in score (M=22.47, SD=4.36). Overall, multivariate analysis indicated that the effect of group on the dependent variables was significant (Wilks' Lambda=0.48, F=7.42, p<0.001, η²=0.307). The results of the Bonferroni test further demonstrated that the happiness and calm groups exhibited superior performance in all cognitive indicators when compared to the fear, anger, and control groups.
Discussion & Conclusion
The findings of this study are consistent with those of previous studies, including those by Soltani & Foroozandeh (2018), Shields et al. (2016), and Zare & Ghorbani (2016). These findings confirm the effective role of positive mood in increasing positive emotions and reducing negative emotions. In accordance with cognitive-emotional theories, such as Beck's theory and congruent processing theory, positive mood, by orienting cognition toward positive thoughts and memories, mitigates negativity bias and enhances emotional and cognitive well-being (Clark et al., 1999; Beck & Emery, 1985). The findings of the study also demonstrated that positive mood significantly improves working memory, cognitive inhibition, and theory of mind. These results align with the findings reported in the studies by Cardi et al. (2019) and Albinet et al. (2012). From a neuropsychological perspective, the activation of the dopaminergic system during positive mood experiences can be attributed to better executive function, which increases motivation, flexibility, and cognitive efficiency (Davidson, 2003; Gray, 1987). Conversely, negative moods, by activating serotonergic and noradrenergic circuits, have been shown to reduce cognitive function, attention, and motivation (Gray, 1987). A notable distinction emerges when contrasting the impact of happiness and calmness: happiness exerts a pronounced effect on enhancing theory of mind, while calmness elicits a marked decline in response time. This discrepancy can be attributed to the differential impact of incentive and inhibitory motivational systems within emotion theories. From a pragmatic standpoint, the recommendation is to utilize positive mood induction interventions in educational settings (e.g., meditation, art, music therapy), professional environments (e.g., positive space design, group activities), and therapeutic contexts (e.g., emotion regulation workshops). The study's limitations include its focus on a specific population (female students) and the use of controlled laboratory conditions. It is recommended that subsequent analyses include a more diverse cohort and encompass natural condition, as well as long-term effects.

Keywords

Main Subjects


Albinet, T.C., Boucard, G., Bouquet, C. A., Audiffren, M. (2012). Processing speed and executive functions in cognitive aging: How to disentangle their mutual relationship Brain and Cognition; 79:1-11.
Alexander, M. P. & Stuss, D. (2000). Disorders of frontal lobe functioning. Executive function Seminaes in Neurology, 20(4);247-437.
Allen, R.J., Schaefer, A, Falcon, T.(2014). Recollecting positive and negative autobiographical                      memories disrupts working memory. Acta Psychological. 151:237–243.
Alloway, T. P., Bibile, V., & Lau, G. (2013). Computerized working memory training: Can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? Computers in Human Behavior, 29(3), 632-638.
Anderson, V., Anderson, P. J., Jacobs, R., Spencer S. M. (2008). Development and assessment of executive function: From preschool to adolescence. In P. Anderson, Vicki; Jacobs, Rani; Anderson (Ed.). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective (pp. 123 – 155).   New York, USA: Psychology Press.
.https://research.monash.edu/en/publications/development-and-assessment-of-executive-function-from-preschool-t

Arjmandnia, A. Sharifi, A. Rostami, R. (1393). The effectiveness of computerized cognitive training on the performance of visual-spatial working memory of students with mathematical problems.3(4), 6-24. In persain 

Ashby, F. G. Isen, A. M., Turken, U. A. (1999). Neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol Rev;106(3):529-50.
Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science, 310(5749), 819-823.
Bakhshipour R, Dezhkam M. (2006). A Confirmatory Factor Analysis Of The Positive Affect And Negative Affect Scales (Panas). Journal of psychology, 9(4). 351- 365.In Persian https://sid.ir/paper/54494/en
Basharpoor S, Amani S, Narimani M. The Effectiveness of Narrative Exposure Therapy on Improving Posttraumatic Symptoms and Interpersonal Reactivity in Patients with Cancer. jrehab 2019; 20 (3) :230-241 .In persian
Beck, A. T.; & Emery, G. (1985). "Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective". New York: Basic Books.
Cardi, V., Leppanen, J., Leslie, M., Esposito, M., Treasure, J. (2019). The use of a positive mood      induction video-clip to target eating behavior in people with bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder: An experimental study. Appetite,131(1). 400-404.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.12.001
 
Clarck, D. A., Beck, A. T., Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory of depression. United State: John Wiley and Sons.
Clark, D. M., Purdon, C. (2004). Cognitive theory and therapy of obsessions compulsions. Journal of contemporary psychotherapy, 8: 90-116
Dadkhah, Yaser, Ahmadi Marzdashti, Negar.2019. Neuropsychological tests. In persian
Davidson, R, J. (2003). Seven sins in the study of emotion: Correctives from affective neuroscience. Brain and Cogn;52(1):129-32.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology. 64: 135- 168.
Dillon, D.G., Pizzagalli, D.A. (2007). Inhibition of action, thought and emotion: A selective            neurobiological review. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(3):99- 114.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.004
Ferguson FJ, Austin EJ. (2010). Associations of trait and ability emotional intelligence with performance on Theory of Mind tasks in an adult sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 49; PP: 414-18
Finch, J.F., Baranik, L.E., Liu, Y., West, S.G. (2012). Physical health, positive and negative affect,               and personality: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Research in Personality 46:537–545.
Ghadiri, Fateme.Jazayeri, ali. Ashayeri, hossein. Ghazi Tabatabaee, mohammad. executive function deficits in patient whith OCD-schiz disorder. Advances in cognitive Science. 2006;8(3) 11-24 In Persian
Gil, K. M., Crason, J. W., Porter, L. S., Scipio, C., Bediako, S. M., Orringer, E. (2004). Daily mood and stress predict pain, health care use and work activity in African-American adults with sickle-cell disease. Health psychol.;23(3):267-74.
Herlitz, A., & Rehnman, J. (2008). Sex differences in episodic memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 52-56.
Hopko, D., Lejuez, C., Daughters, S., Aklin, W., Osborne, A., Simmons, B., et   al.(2006). Construct validity of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART): Relationship with MDMA use by inner-city drug users in residential treatment. J Psychopath & Behav Assess.; 28 (2): 95-101
Kalanthroff, E., Cohen, N., Henik, A. (2013). Stop feeling: Inhibition of emotional interference following stop- signal trials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7:1-7
Kirk, S., Gallagher, G., & Coleman, M. R. (2015). Educating Exceptional Children (14th Ed).         Cengage Learning, Printed in the United States of America.
Kering, A.M., Davison, G.C., Neale, J.M., Johnson, S.L. (2007) Abnormal Psychology Shamsipour H.,          translator. Tehran: Arjomand Publications.
Lecce, S., Bianco, F .(2018). Working memory predicts changes in children’s theory of mind          during middle childhood: A training study. Cognitive Development, 47, 71-81.
Locozi, A. (2016). Effective factors in better performance in students with learning disability. Journal of learning. 23: 45-53.
Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37-45.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011
Mitchell R.L.C., Philips, L.H. (2007). The psychological, neurochemical and functional   neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia. 45(4), 17-629.
Munro, B.A., Weyandt, L.L., Marraccini, M.E., Oster, Dr. (2017). The relationship between nonmedical use of prescription stimulants, executive functioning and academic outcomes. Addictive Behaviors. 65, 250-257.
Nejati, V. Zabihzadeh, A. Mohseni, M. Maleki, S.1392.Social cognition impairmentin patient with major depression; evidence from eye reading tests. Journal of applied psychology.6(5),57-70. In perisan
Ng, E. H. N., Rudner, M., Lunner, T., Pedersen, M. S., & Rönnberg, J. (2013). Effects of noise and working memory capacity on memory processing of speech for hearing-aid users. International Journal of Audiology, 433–441.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response style and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Abnorm Psychol;102(1):20-8.
Peterson, E., Welsh, M. C.(2014). The Development of Hot and Cool Executive Functions in Childhood and Adolescence: Are We Getting Warmer? In S. Goldstein & J. A. Naglieri      (Eds.), Handbook of Executive Functioning (pp. 45 – 69). New York, USA: Springer.https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5_4
Razza, A. R. (2009). Associations among false-belief understanding, executive function, and social competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied DevelopmentalPsychology, 30(3), 332-343.
Shaffer, D.R., Kipp , K.(2007). Development psychology: Childhood and adolescence. California: Wadsworth Publishing, 281, 298-309.
Shields, G.S., Moons, W.G., Tewell, C.A., Yonelinas, A.P.(2016). The effect of negative affect      on cognition: Anxiety, not anger, impairs executive function. Emotion.16(6):792-797.https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000151
Soltani F, Foroozandeh E. Effectiveness of Inducing Positive Affect on Improvement Autobiographical Memory in Depressive Patients. Zanko J Med Sci. 2018; 18 (59) :57-67 . In Persian
Sosic-Vasic Z, Kröner J, Schneider S, Vasic N, Spitzer M, Streb J.(2017) The association between                 parenting behavior and executive functioning in children and young adolescents. Frontiers           in Psychology. 8:472-480.
Storbeck, J., Maswood (2016). Happiness increases verbal and spatial working memory ca pacity where sadness does not: Emotion, working memory and executive control. Cognition and Emotion. 30(5):925-938.  
Taghizadeh T, Nejati V, Mohammadzadeh A, Akbarzade Baghban A. (2014). Paper: Evolution Of Auditory And Visual Working Memory In Primary Schoolaged Children. Journal of Research in rehabilitation sciences, 10(2); 239-249. In Persian https://jrrs.mui.ac.ir/article_16819.html
Wang, Y., Wang, Y. G., Chen, S., Zhu, C., & Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research, 161, 153-161.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018
Watson, D., Clark, L. A. (2006). Extraversion and its positive emotional core. In: Hogan R, Johnson J, Briggs S, editors. Handbook of personality psychology. San Diego: Academic Publication; 1997. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0021550
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief         measures ofpositive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc        Psychol.;54(6):1063. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Zare H, Ghorbani T. Comparing the Effects of Positive and Negative Induced False Memory and True Memory of People With Anxiety-depression Symptoms and Normal Iindividuals. Advances in Cognitive Sciences 2016; 18 (3) :80-89. In persian
Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and Cool Executive Function in Childhood and Adolescence: Development and Plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4),354-360. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2012-31092-006
Zhang, Y., Cole, D.A., Mick, C.R., Lovette, A.J., Garbuck, M.E. (2020). Cognitive reactivity to low positive and high negative affect. Behavior Research and Therapy, 132.
Zuber, S., Ihle, A., Loaiza, V. M., Schnitzspahn, K. M., Stahl, C., Phillips, L. H., Kaller, C. P., Kliegel, M. (2019). Explaining age differences in working memory: The role of updating, inhibition, and shifting. Psychology & Neuroscience, 12(2), 191–208.https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/pne0000151
CAPTCHA Image

Articles in Press, Accepted Manuscript
Available Online from 14 July 2025
  • Receive Date: 24 April 2023
  • Revise Date: 14 December 2024
  • Accept Date: 21 April 2025